Wild animals suppress the spread of socially transmitted misinformation

Edited by Alan Hastings, University of California, Davis, CA; received September 8, 2022; accepted February 7, 2023 March 28, 2023 12...

featured image

Edited by Alan Hastings, University of California, Davis, CA; received September 8, 2022; accepted February 7, 2023

March 28, 2023

120 (14) e2215428120

Significance

Despite the benefits of learning about the world through social ties, social connections also provide a conduit for misinformation. Using underwater camera observatories to record the behavior of foraging coral reef fishes, we find that these animals produce and perceive visual motion cues produced by others, thereby forming dynamic information networks. These networks are surprisingly robust to false alarms that occur when one individual flees in the absence of a true shared threat. By reconstructing visual sensory inputs to each animal, we show that this robustness to misinformation about threats inherits from a specific property of their decision-making strategy: dynamic adjustments in sensitivity to socially acquired information. This property can be achieved through a simple and biologically widespread decision-making circuit.

Abstract

Understanding the mechanisms by which information and misinformation spread through groups of individual actors is essential to the prediction of phenomena ranging from coordinated group behaviors to misinformation epidemics. Transmission of information through groups depends on the rules that individuals use to transform the perceived actions of others into their own behaviors. Because it is often not possible to directly infer decision-making strategies in situ, most studies of behavioral spread assume that individuals make decisions by pooling or averaging the actions or behavioral states of neighbors. However, whether individuals may instead adopt more sophisticated strategies that exploit socially transmitted information, while remaining robust to misinformation, is unknown. Here, we study the relationship between individual decision-making and misinformation spread in groups of wild coral reef fish, where misinformation occurs in the form of false alarms that can spread contagiously through groups. Using automated visual field reconstruction of wild animals, we infer the precise sequences of socially transmitted visual stimuli perceived by individuals during decision-making. Our analysis reveals a feature of decision-making essential for controlling misinformation spread: dynamic adjustments in sensitivity to socially transmitted cues. This form of dynamic gain control can be achieved by a simple and biologically widespread decision-making circuit, and it renders individual behavior robust to natural fluctuations in misinformation exposure.

Get full access to this article

Purchase, subscribe or recommend this article to your librarian.

Data, Materials, and Software Availability

Acknowledgments

We thank S. Hein, T. Gross, and S. Munch for comments and K. Fahimipour for illustrations. A.K.F. was supported by the Research Associateship Program from the National Research Council of the National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Mathematics. This work was supported by NSF grants IOS-1855956 and EF-2222478.

Author contributions

A.K.F. and A.M.H. designed research; A.K.F., M.A.G., and A.M.H. performed research; A.K.F., M.R.C., G.F.H., B.T.M., and A.M.H. contributed new reagents/analytic tools; A.K.F. and A.M.H. analyzed data; and A.K.F., M.A.G., M.R.C., B.T.M., and A.M.H. wrote the paper.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interest.

Supporting Information

References

1

C. Canteloup, W. Hoppitt, E. van de Waal, Wild primates copy higher-ranked individuals in a social transmission experiment. Nat. Commun. 11, 1–10 (2020).

2

A. Flack, M. Nagy, W. Fiedler, I. D. Couzin, M. Wikelski, From local collective behavior to global migratory patterns in white storks. Science 360, 911–914 (2018).

3

S. R. Dall, L. A. Giraldeau, O. Olsson, J. M. McNamara, D. W. Stephens, Information and its use by animals in evolutionary ecology. Trends Ecol. Evol. 20, 187–193 (2005).

4

N. V. Carlson, E. Greene, C. N. Templeton, Nuthatches vary their alarm calls based upon the source of the eavesdropped signals. Nat. Commun. 11, 1–7 (2020).

5

K. Meise, D. W. Franks, J. Bro-Jørgensen, Alarm communication networks as a driver of community structure in African savannah herbivores. Ecol. Lett. 23, 293–304 (2020).

6

K. N. Laland, K. Williams, Social transmission of maladaptive information in the guppy. Behav. Ecol. 9, 493–499 (1998).

7

L. A. Giraldeau, T. J. Valone, J. J. Templeton, Potential disadvantages of using socially acquired information. Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B 357, 1559–1566 (2002).

8

S. L. Lima, Collective detection of predatory attack by social foragers: Fraught with ambiguity? Anim. Behav. 50, 1097–1108 (1995).

9

G. Beauchamp, Determinants of false alarms in staging flocks of semipalmated sandpipers. Behav. Ecol. 21, 584–587 (2010).

10

B. C. Wheeler, K. Hammerschmidt, Proximate factors underpinning receiver responses to deceptive false alarm calls in wild tufted capuchin monkeys: Is it counterdeception? Am. J. Primatol. 75, 715–725 (2013).

11

B. C. Wheeler, Monkeys crying wolf? Tufted capuchin monkeys use anti-predator calls to usurp resources from conspecifics. Proc. R. Soc. B: Biol. Sci. 276, 3013–3018 (2009).

12

T. Flower, Fork-tailed drongos use deceptive mimicked alarm calls to steal food. Proc. R. Soc. B: Biol. Sci. 278, 1548–1555 (2011).

13

S. B. Rosenthal, C. R. Twomey, A. T. Hartnett, H. S. Wu, I. D. Couzin, Revealing the hidden networks of interaction in mobile animal groups allows prediction of complex behavioral contagion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 112, 4690–4695 (2015).

14

M. M. Sosna et al., Individual and collective encoding of risk in animal groups. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 116, 20556–20561 (2019).

15

W. Poel, et al., Subcritical escape waves in schooling fish. Sci. Adv. 8, eabm6385 (2022).

16

M. Moussaïd, D. Helbing, G. Theraulaz, How simple rules determine pedestrian behavior and crowd disasters. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 6884–6888 (2011).

17

G. Markkula, Z. Uludağ, R. M. Wilkie, J. Billington, Accumulation of continuously time-varying sensory evidence constrains neural and behavioral responses in human collision threat detection. PLoS Comput. Biol. 17, e1009096 (2021).

18

M. Ballerini et al., Interaction ruling animal collective behavior depends on topological rather than metric distance: Evidence from a field study. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 105, 1232–1237 (2008).

19

A. M. Hein, M. A. Gil, C. R. Twomey, I. D. Couzin, S. A. Levin, Conserved behavioral circuits govern high-speed decision-making in wild fish shoals. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 115, 12224–12228 (2018).

20

M. A. Gil, A. M. Hein, Social interactions among grazing reef fish drive material flux in a coral reef ecosystem. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 114, 4703–4708 (2017).

21

W. Poel, C. Winklmayr, P. Romanczuk, Spatial structure and information transfer in visual networks. Front. Phys. 623 (2021).

22

R. Harpaz, M. N. Nguyen, A. Bahl, F. Engert, Precise visuomotor transformations underlying collective behavior in larval zebrafish. Nat. Commun. 12, 1–14 (2021).

23

J. M. Kappel, et al., Visual recognition of social signals by a tecto-thalamic neural circuit. Nature 1–7 (2022).

24

P. Domenici, The visually mediated escape response in fish: Predicting prey responsiveness and the locomotor behaviour of predators and prey. Mar. Freshw. Behav. Physiol. 35, 87–110 (2002).

25

I. Temizer, J. C. Donovan, H. Baier, J. L. Semmelhack, A visual pathway for looming-evoked escape in larval zebrafish. Curr. Biol. 25, 1823–1834 (2015).

26

T. W. Dunn et al., Neural circuits underlying visually evoked escapes in larval zebrafish. Neuron 89, 613–628 (2016).

27

K. Bhattacharyya, D. L. McLean, M. A. MacIver, Visual threat assessment and reticulospinal encoding of calibrated responses in larval zebrafish. Curr. Biol. 27, 2751–2762 (2017).

28

A. M. Fernandes et al., Neural circuitry for stimulus selection in the zebrafish visual system. Neuron 109, 805–822 (2021).

29

C. H. Ferreira, M. A. Moita, Behavioral and neuronal underpinnings of safety in numbers in fruit flies. Nat. Commun. 11, 1–10 (2020).

30

P. Ramdya et al., Mechanosensory interactions drive collective behaviour in drosophila. Nature 519, 233–236 (2015).

31

H. Akaike, “Information theory and an extension of the maximum likelihood principle” in Proceedings of 2nd International Symposium on Information Theory (Akademiai Kiado, 1973), pp. 267–281.

32

D. J. Watts, A simple model of global cascades on random networks. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 99, 5766–5771 (2002).

33

J. B. Bak-Coleman, et al., Combining interventions to reduce the spread of viral misinformation. Nat. Hum. Behav. 1–9 (2022).

34

D. Centola, The spread of behavior in an online social network experiment. Science 329, 1194–1197 (2010).

35

S. M. Smirnakis, M. J. Berry, D. K. Warland, W. Bialek, M. Meister, Adaptation of retinal processing to image contrast and spatial scale. Nature 386, 69–73 (1997).

36

M. D. Lazova, T. Ahmed, D. Bellomo, R. Stocker, T. S. Shimizu, Response rescaling in bacterial chemotaxis. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 13870–13875 (2011).

37

M. Usher, J. L. McClelland, The time course of perceptual choice: The leaky, competing accumulator model. Psychol. Rev. 108, 550 (2001).

38

K. Tsetsos, J. Gao, J. L. McClelland, M. Usher, Using time-varying evidence to test models of decision dynamics: Bounded diffusion vs. the leaky competing accumulator model. Front. Neurosci. 6, 79 (2012).

39

M. E. Mazurek, J. D. Roitman, J. Ditterich, M. N. Shadlen, A role for neural integrators in perceptual decision making. Cereb. Cortex 13, 1257–1269 (2003).

40

P. Miller, X. J. Wang, Inhibitory control by an integral feedback signal in prefrontal cortex: A model of discrimination between sequential stimuli. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 103, 201–206 (2006).

41

T. M. Yi, Y. Huang, M. I. Simon, J. Doyle, Robust perfect adaptation in bacterial chemotaxis through integral feedback control. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 97, 4649–4653 (2000).

42

G. De Palo et al., Common dynamical features of sensory adaptation in photoreceptors and olfactory sensory neurons. Sci. Rep. 3, 1–8 (2013).

43

M. Adler, U. Alon, Fold-change detection in biological systems. Curr. Opin. Syst. Biol. 8, 81–89 (2018).

44

O. Shoval et al., Fold-change detection and scalar symmetry of sensory input fields. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 15995–16000 (2010).

45

A. Bizyaeva, A. Franci, N. E. Leonard, Nonlinear opinion dynamics with tunable sensitivity. IEEE Trans. Autom. Control (2022).

46

N. E. Leonard, K. Lipsitz, A. Bizyaeva, A. Franci, Y. Lelkes, The nonlinear feedback dynamics of asymmetric political polarization. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 118, e2102149118 (2021).

47

A. Strandburg-Peshkin et al., Visual sensory networks and effective information transfer in animal groups. Curr. Biol. 23, R709–R711 (2013).

48

N. Hatsopoulos, F. Gabbiani, G. Laurent, Elementary computation of object approach by a wide-field visual neuron. Science 270, 1000–1003 (1995).

49

R. Bogacz, E. Brown, J. Moehlis, P. Holmes, J. D. Cohen, The physics of optimal decision making: A formal analysis of models of performance in two-alternative forced-choice tasks. Psychol. Rev. 113, 700 (2006).

50

S. Miletić, B. M. Turner, B. U. Forstmann, L. van Maanen, Parameter recovery for the leaky competing accumulator model. J. Math. Psychol. 76, 25–50 (2017).

51

D. Matzke, G. D. Logan, A. Heathcote, A cautionary note on evidence-accumulation models of response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm. Comput. Brain Behav. 3, 269–288 (2020).

52

E. Marquez-Legorreta, M. Piber, E. K. Scott, “Visual escape in larval zebrafish: Stimuli, circuits, and behavior” in Behavioral and Neural Genetics of Zebrafish (Elsevier, 2020), pp. 49–71.

53

M. Koyama, A. Kinkhabwala, C. Satou, S. Higashijima, J. Fetcho, Mapping a sensory-motor network onto a structural and functional ground plan in the hindbrain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 108, 1170–1175 (2011).

54

Y. Yao et al., Visual cue-discriminative dopaminergic control of visuomotor transformation and behavior selection. Neuron 89, 598–612 (2016).

Information & Authors

Information

Published in

Go to Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences

Vol. 120 | No. 14
April 4, 2023

Classifications

Copyright

Data, Materials, and Software Availability

Submission history

Received: September 8, 2022

Accepted: February 7, 2023

Published online: March 28, 2023

Published in issue: April 4, 2023

Keywords

  1. misinformation
  2. decision-making
  3. social networks
  4. higher-order interactions
  5. behavioral control

Acknowledgments

We thank S. Hein, T. Gross, and S. Munch for comments and K. Fahimipour for illustrations. A.K.F. was supported by the Research Associateship Program from the National Research Council of the National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Mathematics. This work was supported by NSF grants IOS-1855956 and EF-2222478.

Author Contributions

A.K.F. and A.M.H. designed research; A.K.F., M.A.G., and A.M.H. performed research; A.K.F., M.R.C., G.F.H., B.T.M., and A.M.H. contributed new reagents/analytic tools; A.K.F. and A.M.H. analyzed data; and A.K.F., M.A.G., M.R.C., B.T.M., and A.M.H. wrote the paper.

Competing Interests

The authors declare no competing interest.

Notes

This article is a PNAS Direct Submission.

Authors

Affiliations

Department of Biological Sciences, Florida Atlantic University, Boca Raton, FL 33431

Institute of Marine Sciences, University of California Santa Cruz, Santa Cruz, CA 95060

Department of Ecology & Evolutionary Biology, University of Colorado Boulder, Boulder, CO 80309

Institute of Marine Sciences, University of California Santa Cruz, Santa Cruz, CA 95060

OpenSpace Inc., San Francisco, CA 94108

Benjamin T. Martin

Institute for Biodiversity & Ecosystem Dynamics, University of Amsterdam, 1090 GE Amsterdam, The Netherlands

Department of Computational Biology, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14850

Notes

Metrics & Citations

Metrics

Note: The article usage is presented with a three- to four-day delay and will update daily once available. Due to ths delay, usage data will not appear immediately following publication. Citation information is sourced from Crossref Cited-by service.

Citation statements

Altmetrics

Citations

If you have the appropriate software installed, you can download article citation data to the citation manager of your choice. Simply select your manager software from the list below and click Download.

View Options

Get Access

View options

PDF format

Download this article as a PDF file

DOWNLOAD PDF

Media

Figures

Tables

Other

Read More

COMMENTS

Entertainment